Degrees of Depression
An inquiry among church members that we had used for the
purpose of comparison also revealed that, with the Christians questioned, a
correlation existed between their basic psychological condition at the present
time and the contents that were imparted to them by religious education, that
is, the more positive the notion imparted to them of the “qualities” of
their god and of the traits in the human character was, the better they feel
today; the more sinful man and the more malicious their god appeared to them
from what they had been told, the worse they feel today on the average. As
mentioned above, we had, for reasons of comparability of the various studies,
only evaluated the data obtained from those atheists who, just like the church
members already mentioned, had been brought up in religious faith, then turned
away from the religion that had been inculcated in them and finally left the
church.
In the case of the church opponents who were questioned,
our study revealed that, in contrast to church members, their psychological
condition at the present time does not depend on the specific contents of
religious socialization. For this purpose, we compared—among other data—the
statements relating to the image of God that was imparted to our test persons in
their childhood to their emotional condition at the present time. The test
persons were subdivided into three groups, i.e. groups that had either a
positive, negative, or a neutral notion of God during their childhood. The
F-value of a variance analysis was not significant at the 5% level. In contrast
to the aforementioned study we used for the purpose of comparison, we could not
find any statistically proven correlation between the variables mentioned. We
conclude that the participants in our study had been able to free their minds
and emotions to a great extent from the restraints imposed by religion.
From the first comparison of the results of the studies
examining church members on the one hand and atheists on the other hand, we can
therefore draw the following conclusions:
1. The psychological condition of persons who have kept
religious beliefs in some form or other depends on the form of their religious
education and on their adherence to religious rules, independently of how
closely they feel themselves connected with church and religion
subjectively—in this regard, the statements of the psychologists of religion
cited above are correct, insofar as they speak exclusively of religious persons.
2. They were wrong, however, if they put forth speculative
statements—and that in a quite tendentious manner—with regard to the psychic
condition of atheists. For, as our study proves, a person who underwent
religious socialization and then had the courage and clear mind to break with
religion and church later on has the best chance to live a happier life than any
Christian under statistically comparable conditions. Apart from an atheist
attitude that is based on reason, this requires a clear analysis and
understanding of one’s own past in regard to religion.
In the following, we shall present our findings regarding
the way the church opponents questioned were able to free themselves from their
religious beliefs of the past and describe the correlation between the
determination the test persons had shown in this conflict and their psychic
condition and thinking today.
The Courage to Become
As is to be expected, science plays a central role on the
way from religion by upbringing to atheism. Knowledge gained by means of
observation and logical conclusion is best suited to question the fundamentals
of any religion, that is, the existence of any supernatural being. Ninety-two
percent of our test persons therefore answered the question of whether a gain in
scientific knowledge had played a part in the process of detachment from
religion positively, natural sciences taking first place with 76% of those
questioned. In comparison, only 59% stated that unpleasant experiences with
church institutions had been a decisive factor for them.
Consequently, gaining knowledge seems to be of greater
importance in the process of detachment from religion than unpleasant
occurrences and experiences, which, in turn, can only be evaluated adequately
when, after being judged as to their consequences for the individual, they are
also assessed within overall categories. The first doubts raised by the persons
we questioned were, in 74% of the cases, raised by so-called religious doctrines
(such as the existence of God) and not, as could also be supposed, on the
behavior of religious parents or teachers. We may conclude from this that it is
the breach of the taboo on thinking (that is, of the prohibition of examining
the degree of probability of religious statements) in connection with the gain
in knowledge that is most detrimental to faith and is the most effective form of
protection against mysticism and irrationalism. This is impressively confirmed
by the answers to the question of whether belief in God had come up again at any
time, perhaps in desperate situations, after those questioned had left the
church: 79% of the atheists answered “no,”—that means after their
abandonment they never again showed any inclination to fall back upon the
consolation promised by their former religious faith. Ninety-seven percent of
the atheists we questioned were of the opinion that scientific thinking is
incompatible with religious thinking; moreover, they reject speculation and
irrationalism in not openly religious manifestations as well: 81% reject
astrology, in which God’s influence is replaced by that of the stars; 79%
agree with the statement that “soul and spirit” only exist on the basis of
physiological, e.g. material processes. Consequently, 83% were of the conviction
that there is no life after death whatsoever. Eighty-four percent reject with
reference to the evolution theory any worldly versions of the creation myth,
suggesting a fixed pre-existing plan behind the origin of the vegetable and
animal kingdoms.
Incidentally, one of our results may shed light on how far
the atheists questioned had, during their detachment from religion, moved away
from the “intellectual atmosphere” prevailing in their families. The
families of all of our test persons were, as far as the observation of rituals
is concerned, religious on an average level, but were, compared to the total
population in terms of social statistics, of a more than average academic type;
as was to be expected, in these families, famous personalities, such as Goethe
and King Frederick the Great of Prussia, who, in the eyes of the German educated
bourgeoisie, stand for tolerance in religious matters and a moderately critical
distance towards organized Christianity, enjoyed a mainly positive reputation
(for instance Goethe in 73% of cases, Frederick in 53%). On the other hand, the
“intellectual” attitude in the families towards famous representatives of
the Enlightenment and of sciences probably, in its tendency, corresponded more
to that of the average population. Before they reached the age of ten, 59% of
our test persons did not know
Galileo, 72% did not know anything about Voltaire, and 59% knew nothing about
Darwin, and a determined atheist
such as Marx was judged negatively by 46%. As was also to be expected, all of
these personalities were known to the test persons at the time of the study and
were also judged mainly positively: Galileo by 95%, Voltaire by 86%, Darwin by
93%, Marx by 91%.
Apart from science, sexuality has a central importance in
the detachment from religion. Sixty-six percent of those questioned noted, as
the main point of criticism of religion, “the
suppression of sexual and general self-determination
and of a happy life.” Sixty-six percent also reported that during their
childhood and youth they were imparted the notion that sexuality was sinful,
dirty, and bad, and as a consequence more than 50% of the test persons suffered
from heavy feelings of guilt because of sexual fantasies and activities. While
overcoming religious convictions, 46% succeeded completely, 32% partly in
overcoming those religion-based feelings of guilt (both according to their own
statements). This certainly contributes decisively to the fact that 90% of the
atheists were able to note an increase in their opportunities to enjoy life and
experience happiness compared to the times when they were still religious. The
increase in sexual self-determination is reflected in the increase in general
independence (noted by 87%) and self-confidence (also noted by 87%).
In this context some data from social statistics are also
relevant. The level of education is unusually high among atheists: 39% are
university graduates and another 37% are high school graduates. Their striving
for individual independence is also shown by the fact, for instance, that 60% of
the test persons are not married (compared to only 40% of the total population
of the Federal Republic of Germany) and a further 13% are divorced.
Moreover, the study also revealed differences regarding the
degree of self-determination and the proneness to depression among the atheist
population. In the course of the evaluation of the data we were able to
determine some reasons for statistically interpretable deviations between the test
persons. For example, we should, on the one hand, point out that the statistical
comparison between the depression values of men and women did not render any
differences. This result is especially noteworthy because in the average
population women suffer far more frequently from depression than men. The
evaluation of our questionnaires led to the result that women, compared to men,
had suffered additional disadvantages in the course of their religious
education. Thirty-one percent of the men and none of the women noted advantages;
67% of the women noted disadvantages due to their sex in religious education.
This sex-linked difference in religious education is statistically highly
significant (x2 = 31.94; a = .000). We are able to prove that most of our test
persons who—compared to others—have been victims of greater impairment were
able to make up for it by greater efforts during the detachment from religion
and that this is why they did not show any higher depression values than men at
the time of the study. There was no significant F-value with respect to sex
using variance analysis. A small group of women, however, reported that they did
not fight against the sex-linked specific role they were expected to adopt, as
was imparted to them during their religious education. These women showed
significantly higher depression values, which confirms our basic hypothesis that
the extent of the individual analysis and assessment of one’s religious past
will decide to a considerable degree the extent of the present capability of
enjoying happiness.
The overwhelming majority of the test persons referred to
themselves as militant atheists (74%); only a few of them were rather hesitant
and undetermined in their opposition to the church. We examined the possibility
of a statistical correlation between this rather placid attitude towards the
church and the tendency of these test persons to fall back upon so-called
religious coping strategies, e.g. consolation promised by the church. While 74%
of the militant atheists do not remember situations when they would have wished
to pray again, this is only the case with 61% of the nonmilitant atheists. This
is a statistically significant result (x2 = 10.66; a = .03). Thus we
found—apart from the extent of the assessment of one’s own religious
past—a second criterion for the statistical prediction of the psychic
condition of atheists: resoluteness in their opposition to religion and church.
Perception vs. Reality
In public, the image of an unhappy and dismal atheist who
is haunted by inner doubts and fears and is already paying dearly for his
opposition to religion in this life is often conveyed. This image is not
necessarily—and not always—wrong, but it is certainly wrong when, as we were
able to prove in our study, atheists who have gone through a religious
upbringing later on succeed in reconquering all spheres of life we described
above, which had previously been occupied by the church.
In order to conclude this short summary of the evaluation
of the questionnaires (the original study comprises more than 500
pages—further details can be furnished on request) we should like, once again,
to point out the most important results: contrary to the tendentious assertions
put up by numerous studies on the psychology of religion, simply taking
advantage of the statistically small number of atheists compared to the
relatively great number of strictly religious persons, atheists are less prone
to depression than religious persons. Their psychic condition differs most
impressively from those who, though quite obviously with a guilty conscience, do
not keep the church’s rules, but never seriously analyzed their own religious
education and their obviously persistent secret, religion-based convictions.
There is a less distinct difference between atheists and strictly religious
persons who unbrokenly stick to religious prescriptions and therefore are less
depressed by feelings of guilt than “lukewarm” Christians. But atheists also
have an advantage over the hard core of believers with respect to their
depression values—although the difference is not so great.
The study we have presented here in short summary is, to
our knowledge, the only one worldwide to examine, with due scientific scrutiny,
a population of resolute atheists, allowing a comparison of this group with
believers by means of a standardized measure. Doubtlessly there is considerable
need for further investigation in this field, especially regarding the process
of detachment from religion, but such a project—at least in Germany—will not
meet with much support from public institutions, quite contrary to inquiries
conducted among believers. Further in-depth analysis and verification of our
results in international and transcultural comparisons would also be very
desirable. We do hope to have given an impulse in this direction with our
study.
No comments:
Post a Comment